Western Diamondback Rattlesnake Warning

A Western Diamondback Rattlesnake communicating with me in the same way it does with any of the many predators that it fears. He stands up as tall as he can, hissing, rattling, and making the idea of coming any closer a decidedly bad idea.

What it isn’t doing: showing aggression. This behavior, even though it may seem scary (as it is intended to), is not a sign of “attack”, but a small animal’s best attempt at preserving its own life. For some reason, humans have a very hard time understanding this, and the incredibly simple way for everyone to leave safely: walk away. After a few photos, that’s what I did. Thanks for the heads up, snake.

Arizona Ridgenosed Rattlesnake

An Arizona Ridgenosed Rattlesnake from the southeastern corner of Arizona. I photographed this young snake back in 2014 (that’s how behind I am in photos).

At this point, assuming she hasn’t met an end by fire, drought, bear, poacher, or any of the other ways a small snake can enter the carbon cycle, she is an older adult. She may have given birth every couple or few years, and still has a lot of life to live. Rattlesnakes can live for decades, continually learning to make better use of their environment and home range.

Mojave Rattlesnake near Phoenix

A Mojave Rattlesnake near Phoenix, Arizona last year. This relatively young snake. While the old idea of counting rattle segments to tell the age of a snake isn’t accurate, much can still be learned. For instance, this snake still has its prebutton intact, which it was born with – its first shed skin revealing the full button. Then two articulated segments, and a the basal segment mean this snake has shed three times in its life. Given the strong and steady taper of segments and very healthy weight, and time of year it was found (late in the monsoon): this snake had just had its first birthday. As usual, it’s not green … and not “aggressive”. These timid snakes, like any other rattlesnake … or any other animal … will do all they can to prevent their own death. Add in a lifetime of hearing from everyone around that they are aggressive and chase you down on sight, not a small amount of social media distortion, and a sprinkle of personal identity reliance on contrived threats, and you have the most feared animal in the West.

But if this is you … it’s not your fault. Your brain can lie to you. In fact, with things you may fear, it can do so quite effectively. So while many stories of Mojaves chasing someone through the desert are flat-out lies, many are not. The reason: just like the snake, you are programmed for survival. In some cases, this means adding a few exclamation points to perception and memory.

And if you don’t buy that your brain can lie to you: open a new tab and look up any optical illusion. Right there in front of you: your brain is lying. Then, read these publications:

1. Riskind, J. H., Moore, R., & Bowley, L. (1995).

The looming of spiders: The fearful perceptual distortion of movement and menace. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(2), 171–178. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0005796794E0023C

This paper explores how fear increases the perceived motion and menace of feared objects such as spiders and snakes. Fearful individuals overestimated the movement of spiders/snakes, evidencing distorted space/time perception under phobic conditions.

2. Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2003).

The malicious serpent: Snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(1), 5–9. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1111/1467-8721.01211

This article theorizes that fear of snakes activates an “evolved fear module” in the brain, causing rapid and automatic responses to snake stimuli. It suggests deep-rooted neurocognitive pathways shape the perception of danger.

3. Teachman, B. A., Gregg, A. P., & Woody, S. R. (2001).

Implicit associations for fear-relevant stimuli among individuals with snake and spider fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(2), 226–235. https://www.academia.edu/download/49240316

Using implicit association tests, this paper reveals distorted cognitive processing of snake stimuli in phobic individuals. It supports the idea that fear biases long-term memory and perception of threat magnitude.

4. Soares, S. C. (2010).

Fear commands attention: Snakes as the archetypal fear stimulus? [Doctoral dissertation]. Karolinska Institutet. https://openarchive.ki.se/articles/thesis/Fear_commands_attention_snakes_as_the_archetypal_fear_stimulus_/26901778/1/files/48943789.pdf

This dissertation offers detailed experiments demonstrating that snakes command disproportionate attentional resources, even in peripheral vision, leading to spatial distortion and overestimated danger.

Arizona Ridgenosed Rattlesnakes With Babies

An Arizona Ridgenosed Rattlesnake with her newborns.

Like many snakes, including all vipers in the U.S., these rattlesnakes do not lay eggs. They give live birth to young, which stay with the mother for a period of time afterward. Once they’ve shed skin for the first time, around ten days later, they will disperse. In the meantime, these little ones spend time with their moth, following her lead on their first experiences with a big, dangerous world.

Blacktailed Rattlesnake Den

A Blacktailed Rattlesnake hiding out just outside its den. It took a few years to zero in on the exact spot, surveying the area in early spring and late fall and tracking where snakes were found in early egress. Eventually, there it was – with just two snakes in it to start, but the start of many more to come. The snake wasn’t disturbed, and due to the location, very likely may have never seen a human before.